Thursday 21 March 2013

Male Victims of Domestic Violence

Today has been a most encouraging day and I have found, not one but two, academic studies concerning male domestic violence victims.  As I read both, it seemed my story was mirrored.  To also identify the journey my emotions, my health, my feeling of helplessness within those articles gave me a greater sense of encouragement.

The first paper was  'A Closer Look at Men Who Sustain Intimate Terrorism by Women (2010 Hines & Douglas)

www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2913504/

and the second appears in New Male Studies Volume 2 No.1

http://newmalestudies.com/OJS/index.php/nms/index

Both articles originate from North America and I'm not aware of any work similar to them being produced in England.

I sent an email to all the authors and was absolutely thrilled to receive a reply from all.  The shorter article is
'Male Victims of Domestic Violence'  and both Donald G Dutton and Katherine R. White have kindly allowed me to include in on this blog:

MALE VICTIMS OF DOMESTIC VIOLENCE (2013 Dutton & White)



Intimate partner violence (IPV) or domestic violence (DV) is often framed as a “woman’s issue” or
“violence against women” generating the perception of males involved in violent relationships as the
aggressor and more capable of inflicting injury or causing harm to their partner. Due to this set of
beliefs called the “gender paradigm”, male victims are often met with disbelief or suspicion when
they attempt to gain protection from a female partner, or access services. Male victims may also report
difficulty in locating services specific to their needs, as help lines or shelters are targeted exclusively
towards female victims. These issues and the implications for male victims will be discussed.

Key Words: domestic violence, male victims, intimate partner violence, gender paradigm

The child who I saw being hit by his mother is three times more likely to become violent
in intimate relationships than a child who was not hit. The moment that he
hits a woman, it is legislated that he be taken out of the context of his biography and
into an automatic legal process in which he will be held absolutely accountable for
any violence he committed. He will be defined as a product of patriarchy, and his
masculine privilege will account for the sole source of his aggression.
Linda Mills, Insult to Injury (2003, p. 3)

The stereotype invoked when one mentions “domestic violence” is of a bullying, domineering man
who is hyper-reactive to jealousy and has a drinking problem. He threatens, assaults and verbally
intimidates a non-violent woman-victim. If you ask college students for examples of domestic violence
perpetrators, you likely get OJ Simpson or Chris Brown as an answer. Although we may like to
believe that such simplistic stereotypes are held only by the uninformed, alas, it is not true. Academics
who would bristle at any stereotyping of women or minorities adhere to the “gender paradigm”;
that all domestic violence is male perpetrated against hapless female victims, in order to
preserve “patriarchy” -male dominance of women. For examples of such thinking see any work by
Russell or Emerson Dobash (Dobash & Dobash, 1979; 1988), Walter Dekeseredy (Dekeseredy, 2011;
DeKeseredy & Schwartz, 2003) or Mollie Dragiewicz (Dragiewicz, 2008; Dragiewicz & Lindgren,
2009), amongst others. The theory driving this view is a Marxist-feminism perspective developed by
Catherine Mackinnon (MacKinnon, 1989) and posits that “sexuality is to feminism what work is to
marxism (sic)” (p. 3), hence domestic violence in which a man hits a woman is defined as “violence
against women”– plural– a political act. There is no such term for when a woman hits a man and it
is rarely used when a woman hits a woman (e.g., Lie, Schilit, Bush, Montague, & Reyes, 1991). The
latter examples are more likely to be seen as psychologically driven actions. When data began to surface
about female intimate partner violence (IPV) from the national survey data of professor Murray
Straus (Straus, 1980), it was quickly dismissed as inconsequential violence, in Michael Johnson’s
term, “common couple violence” (Johnson, 1995) that was bilateral and where the woman was acting
in self-defence (Saunders, 1986; 1988; 2002). Now we know that women assault non-violent male
partners more frequently than men assault non-violent female partners.
This “gender paradigm” was consistently reinforced by numerous studies on “male perpetrators”
and “female victims”, the former drawn from court-mandated treatment groups (e.g., Dutton,
1995b; Gondolf, 1999; Saunders, 1992) and the latter from women’s shelters (e.g., Johnson, 2008). In
short, samples selected on the basis of their own perpetration or victimization and not representative
of the community (Straus, 1992b). True believers operating within the gender paradigm do not question
the generalizability of such samples; selected by a system that was already operating on the assumption
that men were sole perpetrators and women were victims. Johnson, for example,
concluded that men were the only perpetrators of what he called “intimate terrorism” (Johnson &
Leone, 2005), that is, the use of intimate partner violence (IPV) for instrumental purposes. He came
to that conclusion by interviewing women in shelters, taking their descriptions of violence against
them as veridical and not asking them about their own use of violence. As Johnson put it “I chose
one question to determine whether the husband and/or wife had been violent, as reported by the
wife” (Johnson, 2008, p. 20). The implication of this research choice was that Johnson trusted only
women’s versions of events and based his entire analysis of IPV on this version. Johnson made no
assessment of whether the reports he obtained under these conditions were veridical or self-serving

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
6

inflations of victimization or enhanced with stories overheard from other shelter clients. He could
not know. His methodology reinforced the view that women were the sole and passive victims of domestic
violence. Is it any wonder then that “intimate terrorism (IT)” depicted by this sample appears
to be solely male perpetrated, and that female IPV involves “violent resistance” (i.e., self defence) in
women. Women in shelters are a sample that has been selected because of extreme IPV generated
towards them (Straus, 1992b). If you change the sample, however, the conclusion changes. For example,
a study by Graham-Kevan and Archer (2003) found that as the authors put it “...the “maleness”
of intimate terrorism may well be an artifact of the sampling procedure used. Indeed, if the shelter
data is omitted IT shows sexual symmetry” (p. 1261). Eighty percent of the male intimate terrorists
found were reported by the shelter sample, even though it constituted only 17% of the their entire
sample, (i.e., it was not found in other samples). LaRoche (2005) assessed “intimate terrorism” in
the data from the 2004 Canadian National Social Survey, that assessed power dynamics as well as
IPV. In those national data, 4.2% of women and 2.6% of men reported being victimized by intimate
terrorism. A study of men seeking help from IPV victimization (Hines & Douglas, 2010) found IT
patterns were gender reversed for this group compared with a women’s shelter group (more about
this below). Any study that assesses gender prevalence of IT with a non-shelter sample produces
very different results from Johnson. Should it come as a surprise that if you ask only questions about
victimization from a pre-selected victim group, you obtain very skewed and misleading results? As
far back as 1992 Murray Straus had reported (Straus, 1992b) that shelter samples had 11 times the violence
perpetrated against them as did community samples of women.
It is not only a sampling issue however, it is also an issue of not inquiring about women’s violence.
By way of comparison with Johnson’s one sided approach, Renee McDonald and her colleagues
asked about violence both toward and by women in shelters (McDonald, Jouriles, Tart, &
Minze, 2009). When asked about their own use of violence 67% of these women reported using an
act of severe violence themselves against their partner. The women’s own violence was an important
determinant of child behavior problems. As the authors put it “men’s severe IPV seldom occurs in
the absence of other forms of family violence” (p. 94), these other forms included both partner-child
aggression and mother-child aggression. This finding runs counter to the stereotype of wife assault
of a non-violent women because it was a rare study that avoided the “one sided question” issue. We
will return to it below.
The mother’s use of aggression (i.e., physical child abuse) contributed to the child’s externalizing
(i.e.,acting-out) problems, especially if the child was a boy. Furthermore, in a community
sample of 1,615 dual parent households, children were 2.5 times more likely to be exposed to IPV by
their mother than by their father (McDonald, Jouriles, Tart, & Minze, 2009). Also, in the huge U.S.
National Survey on Child Maltreatment (718, 948 investigations of child abuse), the more frequent
perpetrators were biological mothers (58%: Gaudioisi, 2006). Boys are most at risk for physical violence
from their mothers. To paraphrase Linda Mills opening quote, this mother-generated externalizing
heightens the chance of later use of IPV, at which point, the man is now a “batterer” and a
product of patriarchy.

the reporting issue

One reason that intimate partner violence toward men is underestimated is that men are less likely
to view the IPV as a crime or to report it to police. Men have been asked in surveys if they had been
assaulted and if so, had they reported it to police. In a 1985 survey, less than 1% of men who had
been assaulted by their wife had called police (Stets & Straus, 1992). In that same survey men as-

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
7
saulted by their wife were less likely to hit back than were wives assaulted by their husband. Men
were also far less likely to call a friend or relative for help (only 2%). As we shall see below, it is not
the case that these assaults were inconsequential. Male socialization diminishes the likelihood of
reaching out for help (Goldberg, 1979). Historically, men who were victims of assault by their wives
were made into objects of social derision (Davidson, 1977), a practice in medieval Europe called
charivari that involved riding the victim around town, seated backwards on a donkey and punching
his genitals (Dutton, 1995a). Men are socialized to bury problems under a private veil (Goldberg,
1979), including being the object of abuse from female partners. It is of note that men’s reports on
surveys of victimization by IPV is less than female reports of perpetration (Desmarais, Reeves,
Nicholls, Telford, & Fiebert, 2012a; 2012b). Either the women are bragging or the men are in denial,
or both.

the one Sided Question issue

We showed above how Johnson’s use of a one-sided type of question (i.e., asking women in shelters
only about violence done to them) led to his erroneous conclusions about “intimate terrorism”. This
problem has also afflicted surveys of IPV that inquire only about victimization. The National Survey
of Violence Against Women (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000) asked a representative US sample about
“crime victimization.” Of course, the use of that filter suppresses reporting because it assumes respondents
will define the abuse as a crime. Straus (1999) has shown that removing this filter by asking
about specific behaviors used in response to conflict increases reporting rates of abuse by a factor of
16, because it asks respondents to simply endorse a specific act (in terms of whether the individual
did it or had it done to them) rather than define the act depicted as abuse. However, it also produces
gender rates of IPV that are identical, leading to criticism of the scale by those who wish to screen
out evidence contradicting the gender paradigm (Straus, 1992a). Apart from filters, there is another
serious problem, with asking one-sided questions about IPV; bilateral IPV is missed.
Bilateral IPV is where both members of the couple use violence. Five large scale surveys that
asked about both victimization and perpetration found that the most common form of IPV was bilateral
(two way IPV), matched for level of severity (see Table 1). Of the remaining unilateral cases,
70% were perpetrated by women, only 30% by men (Stets & Straus, 1989; Whitaker, Haileyesus,
Swahn, & Saltzman, 2007). This finding has the following implication for one sided victimization
surveys; about 75% of the women reporting victimization were also perpetrators. This is easily derived
by taking the number of women who report victimization on a survey as a denominator (i.e., those
who would have reported victimization to a one-sided survey) and those who report bilateral perpetration
as the numerator (i.e., those who reported perpetration as well as victimization). The actual
results produced are 84% for cohabiting couples and 73% for married couples (Stets & Straus, 1989).
That is the percentage of victimized women who were also perpetrators. In the Whittaker et al. survey
(2007), this percentage was 77%. For men in the Stets and Straus study, the corresponding percentages
were 58.5% (married) and 59.6% (co-habiting). It’s less relevant for men because no surveys
have ever solely focused on IPV victimization in men. This recalculation also shows how bilaterality
of IPV was missed by one sided inquiries.

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
8


effects on Male Victims

The gender paradigm stereotype also holds that female violence is less serious, only what Johnson
calls “common couple violence” (Johnson, 1995). In fact, the data again say something else. It was
simply that earlier research was driven by a paradigm that avoided asking the right questions of men.
When these questions are asked, the results are surprising. An emergency clinic in Philadelphia
found that 12.6 percent of all male patients over a thirteen-week period (N=866) were victims of domestic
violence (Mechem, Shofer, Reinhard, Horing, & Datner, 1999). These patients reported having
been kicked, bitten, punched, or choked by female intimate partners in 47 percent of cases, and in

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
9

37 percent of cases reported a weapon being used against them. The authors observe that the numbers
would have been higher except they had to stop counting after midnight and screened out “major
trauma” cases, which could have upped the proportion injured by female partners. Note that many
emergency clinics ask women but not men about potential domestic violence origins for injuries.
An emergency clinic study in Ohio found that 72 percent of men admitted with injuries from spousal
violence had been stabbed (Vasquez & Falcone, 1997). The authors report that burns obtained in intimate
violence were as frequent for male victims as for female victims.
Coker et al. (2002) reanalyzed data from the NVAW survey (N=6,790 women and 7,122 men)
to assess associations between physical, sexual, and psychological abuse and current and long-term
physical and psychological effects in men and women. Results indicated that psychological and
physical abuse were associated with much the same outcomes and had similar effects for men and
women. The authors cautioned that it is possible that male victims were also perpetrators and their
mental health status resulted from inflicting abuse rather than from being victimized. Interestingly,
they did not present this hypothesis for women.
The reanalysis of the Canadian General Social Survey data by Laroche (2005), based on a
sample of 25,876, also strongly refutes the idea that males do not suffer ill effects from intimate partner
violence. It is of interest that, though not all “victim” data in that survey were available for men,
what was available indicated great similarity in male and female victimization. Laroche (2005) reported
that 83% of men who “feared for their life” did so because they were unilaterally terrorized
by their female partner compared to the 77% of women who were unilaterally terrorized. Of the terrorized
men, 80% reported having their everyday activities disrupted (compared to 74% for terrorized
women), 84% received medical care (the same rate as for terrorized women), and 62% sought psychological
counseling (63% for women: see Table 8, p. 16). Hence, in an immense nationally representative
sample, victim reactions for abused men were virtually identical to those of abused women.
It was simply that earlier research was driven by a paradigm that avoided asking the right questions
of men.
Men who are victims of IPV exhibit negative psychological symptoms, in addition to possible
physical injury (although, on average men are less likely to sustain injury compared to women:
Archer, 2000). In a multi-site study of 3,461 male university students, IPV victimization was associated
with Posttraumatic Stress (PTS) symptoms. With more severe IPV victimization associated with
a greater severity of PTS symptoms (Hines, 2007). Additional support of this finding was reported
in a clinical sample of men. Men who had sustained common couple violence were more likely to
meet the clinical cut-off for PTSD compared to men who had not sustained IPV (8.2%; 2.1%), but
the group with the highest rates of PTSD were men who sustained intimate terrorism (57.9%: Hines
& Douglas, 2011)
After years of studies of battered women drawn from transition houses for women, a set of
studies were finally done on men seeking help for IPV victimization. Using a sample of men contacting
the New Hampshire domestic violence hotline, the only one in North American for men,
Denise Hines (Hines, Brown, & Dunning, 2007) finally provided a view of male victims of IPV. Hines
and Douglas (2010) reported that in this male victim sample, 20% had experienced extreme violence
(e.g., choking, using a knife, burning with scalding water, targeting of their genitals) during attacks,
and that 95% of the female perpetrators used controlling acts consistent with Intimate Terrorism

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
10
(e.g., death threats, threats to the family pet, display of weapons, smashing things, threats of using
the criminal justice system–calling the police and lodging a domestic violence complaint, using the
court system to obtain sole custody, etc.). Seventy eight percent of the men were injured (Hines),
2007 sustaining on average eleven injuries. Hines and Douglas (2011) used a community sample as
controls. In the community sample they found that CCV was the most common form of IPV. However,
with the sample of help-seeking men, “a very different picture emerged” (p. 51). Female partners
of these men used 5-6 times the frequency of physical and severe psychological aggression of the
men themselves (by the men’s reports) and 5-6 times the controlling behaviors. Rates of their own
use of IPV by the help seeking men were similar to those reported by shelter women in the few studies
that reported these data (e.g., McDonald et al., 2009; Hines & Douglas, 2010,). They constituted a
virtual mirror image (i.e., gender reversal) of the female victim samples reported by Johnson. When
they sought help from a local DV program, 64% of these abused men were told that they were the
‘’real batterer.” The gender paradigm never acknowledges the existence of male victims, in part, because
shelters for men (and therefore, samples of male victims) have never existed.

the Criminal Justice Solution

Criminal justice practice requires a perpetrator and a victim, that’s how the world is divided, so it is
no surprise that bilaterally violent couples will be divided in this manner by police intervention.
Deborah Capaldi and her colleagues performed the essential study on this matter (Capaldi et al.,
2009). As part of the ongoing Oregon Youth Survey, Capaldi et al. assessed 150 couples in late adolescence
and early adulthood. Bilaterally violent couples whose level of IPV rose during one event,
called the police who then arrested the man (in 85% of cases). It should be pointed out that the
man’s level of aggression was higher on that incident, but the IPV pattern preceding that event had
been mutual and matched for severity. Brown (2004) found that men were more likely than women
to be arrested and prosecuted for IPV. For example, in cases where neither partner sustained injury,
men were over 15 times more likely than females to be charged (61% vs. 3.8%). Henning and Renauer
(2005) found that men were more likely to be arrest compared to women, even when other factors
were controlled (e.g., prior arrests). Men also faced harsher legal ramifications post-arrest, in this
sample 85% of men, but only 53.5% of women who were arrested were prosecuted (Henning & Renauer,
2005).
Men that were suspected of being perpetrators of violence are treated more harshly by the
criminal justice system, but so are men who reach out for protection. In reviewing current research,
Russell (2012) found that men were less likely to receive a protection order from their female partner.
This supports the claim that male victimization is not taken as seriously in courts, as these men were
not seen as requiring protection at the same rate as women. Police and criminal justice professionals
are steeped in the gender paradigm, it is part of police training. When these biases are added to the
male reluctance to report IPV, it is easy to see why any research based on criminal justice statistics
is misleading; it underestimates both bilaterality and female perpetration.

Perceptions of Domestic Violence

Studies of lay persons (Sorenson & Taylor, 2005) and psychologists (Follingstad, DeHart, & Green,

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
11
2004) reveal that the stereotype created by the gender paradigm is pervasive; both groups view an
identical action when committed by a man as more abusive and more likely to require police intervention.
Male victimization is not viewed to be as serious as female victimization. Regardless of injuries
sustained, or other negative outcomes, society views IPV perpetrated by a women towards a
man as less dangerous and less potentially harmful to the victim (see, White & Dutton, 2013).
Gender stereotypes profoundly affect our perceptions of the seriousness and preferred outcomes
of domestic violence. A random-digit dialed survey of 3, 679 residents of Los Angeles (Sorenson
& Taylor, 2005) found that actions are more likely to be considered abusive by the general public
if performed by males. This was true across all sociodemographic groups and includes what we normally
would call “psychological abuse”, not just physical abuse. Furthermore, respondents deemed
the same action when performed by a man as actionable (i.e., “should be illegal”). This included acts
such as “punch” and “pressure for sex.”
Of perhaps greater concern is that Follingstad et al. (2004) found that this gender bias was
also true of psychologists. Two scenarios describing the context and psychologically abusive behaviors
with the genders reversed were given to 449 clinicians (56% male), with a median age of 52. Psychologists
rated male perpetrated behaviors as more abusive and severe than a female’s use of the
same actions. Contextual factors (e.g., frequency/intent/perception of recipient) did not affect this
tendency. The items rated as significantly more abusive if performed by a man included “made to
account for whereabouts at all times”, “would not allow to look at members of same sex”, “threatened
to have committed to an institution” and “made derogatory comments.” The significance on these
items was independent of the sex of the psychologist. In both the Sorenson and Follingstad studies,
identical behaviors were more likely to be judged as abusive when done by a male to a female.
As Follingstad et al. concluded, “the stereotypical association between physical aggression
and males appears to extend to an association of psychological abuse and males” (p. 447). Unfortunately
this sometimes leads to serious problems. Coontz, Lidz and Mulvey (1994) found that clinical
predictions of dangerousness made in psychiatric emergency rooms consistently underestimated
female dangerousness. Predictions that a male would not be violent were correct 70% of the time,
but for females, they were correct only 55% of the time. Skeem and his colleagues (2005) had 147 clinicians
assess 680 patients in a psychiatric emergency room for risk of future violence. Mental health
professionals of both genders were “particularly limited in their ability to assess female patients’ risk
of future violence” (p. 173). In fact the false negative rate for female patients (i.e., the rate at which
one was judged to be low risk but subsequently re-offended) was double that of male patients. The
criterion for violence was physical violence: the patient had to have been reported to have “laid hands
on another person with the intent to harm him or her, or had threatened someone with a weapon in
hand” (p. 178). This finding was true across all professional groups and was unrelated to type of violence.
That is, the finding occurred for general violence and for severe violence. In the MacArthur
Risk Assessment study of psychiatric patients released into the public, Robins et al. (1987) found
that women were just as likely as men to be violent during the first year after discharge. Robins and
her colleagues attributed the underestimation of women’s violence to it being less visible “since it
occurs disproportionately in the home with family members” (p. 182).
Changes towards societal acceptance of male- and female-perpetrated IPV have moved at a
discrepant rate. Over a 26-year period (from 1968 to 1994) the approval of male-perpetrated violence
towards a female partner decreases significantly, from 20% to 10%, whereas rates of approval of female-
perpetrated IPV remained consistent (at 22%) over this same period of time (Straus, Kaufman
Kantor & Moore, 1997). The authors state that efforts condemning female-perpetrated violence did
not exist to a similar degree as efforts to reduce male-perpetrated violence.

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
12

the Custody issue

The gender paradigm has simply played havoc with fairness in custody decisions. In books designed
for custody assessors, men have been portrayed as the only parent requiring assessment for violence
potential to their children, that abusers (who are only men) will lie during these assessments and
that abusive men will be especially litigious in court (Bancroft & Silverman, 2002; Jaffe, Johnston,
Crooks, & Bala, 2008; Jaffe, Lemon, & Poisson, 2003). Jaffe et al. (2003) claim “30-60% of children
whose mothers had experienced abuse were themselves likely to be abuse” (p. 30). The actual overlap
is about 4-6% and that is only when spanking is counted as physical child abuse (Appel & Holden,
1998). Jaffe et al. generalized his conclusions from a women’s shelter sample, Bancroft from a court
mandated group of male perpetrators. Evaluators reading these books will be primed to suspect only
the male and to expect that male to lie. It’s a blueprint for a witch hunt and is not supported by the
data. The present senior author has strongly critiqued these specific papers (Dutton, 2005; 2006;
Dutton, Hamel, & Aaronson, 2010; Dutton & Nicholls, 2005). It is appalling that such a wrongheaded
view should impact on custody decisions. In view of the fact that there is not a shred of scientific evidence
to support the gender paradigm misinformation, these writers should be exhorted to recant
and set the record straight.
A study of 135,573 child maltreatment investigations conducted by Health Canada, and published
by the National Clearing House on Family Violence (Trocme et al., 2001) examined physical
abuse, sexual abuse, neglect, emotional maltreatment and “multiple categories” within the general
population. Cases of alleged abuse were further divided into substantiated, suspected, and unsubstantiated
categories. Substantiation rates did not, in general, vary by gender of perpetrator and
ranged from 52 to 58%. Compared to biological fathers, biological mothers were found more likely
to perpetrate child physical abuse (47% vs. 42%), neglect their children (86% vs. 33%), engage in
emotional maltreatment (61% vs. 55%), and contribute to multiple categories (66% vs. 36%). Biological
fathers are more likely perpetrators of child sexual abuse (15% vs. 5%).
A second study, using an even larger sample of 718,948 reported cases of child abuse, was
conducted by the United States Administration for Children and Families (Gaudioisi, 2006) and reported
that, in 2005, women (58% of the child abuse perpetrators) were upwards of 1.3 times more
likely to abuse their children than were men. When acting alone, biological mothers were twice as
likely to abuse their children as were biological fathers, and biological mothers were the main perpetrators
of child homicide. Also, as described above, McDonald et al. (2006) found that risks of
child exposure to violence were 2.5 times higher for female- (mother-)perpetrated violence than
male- (father-)perpetrated violence. Thus, again, the best research data, from the largest and most
rigorous studies tell a very different story from that related by Jaffe et al. and Bancroft.

Conclusion

Both male victims and male perpetrators have a more difficult experience in the aftermath of IPV.
Male perpetrators receive harsher legal penalties, and are judged as more capable of inflicting injury
or instilling fear in their female partner. This is true even when they have been part of a bilateral IPV
pattern. Male victims also fare worse when attempting to access services, as males are more likely to

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
13
be labelled the aggressor and to be treated with suspicion and injuries they have sustained are likely
to be minimized. Custody assessments are misdirected, focusing on the male as the sole source of
threat to children for physical abuse. A major revision of our thinking is required, one that is empirically
based and can to alter an emotionally tinged stereotype.

references

Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual partners: A meta-analytic review.
Psychological Bulletin, 126(5), 651-680.
Appel, A. E., & Holden, G. W. (1998). The co-occurence of spouse abuse and physical child abuse:
A review and appraisal. Journal of Family Violence, 12(4), 578-599.
Bancroft, L., & Silverman, J. G. (2002). The batterer as parent: Addressing the impact of domestic
violence on family dynamics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Brown, G. A. (2004). Gender as a factor in the response of the law-enforcement system to violence
against partners. Sexuality and Culture, 8(3-4), 3-139.
Caetano, R., Vaeth, P. A. C., Ramisetty-Mikler, S. (2008). Intimate partner violence victims and
perpetrator characteristics among couples in the United States. Journal of Family Violence,
23(6), 507-518.
Capaldi, D. M., Wu Shortt, J., Kim, H. K., Wilson, J., Crosby, L., & Tucci, S. (2009). Official incidents
of domestic violence: Types, injury and associations with nonofficial couple aggression.
Violence and Victims, 24(4), 502-519.
Coker, A. L., Davis, K. E., Arias, I., Desai, S., Sanderson, M., Brandt, H. M., Smith, P. H. (2002).
Physical and mental health effects of intimate partner violence for men and women. American
Journal of Preventative Medicine, 23(4), 260-268.
Coontz, P. D., Lidz, C. W., & Mulvey, E. P. (1994). Gender and the assessment of dangerousness in
the psychiatric emergency room. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 17(4), 369-
376.
Davidson, T. (1977). Wife beating: A recurring phenomenon throughout history. In M. Roy (Ed.),
Battered women: A psychosociological study of domestic violence (pp. 1-23). New York, NY:
Van Nostrand.
Dekeseredy, W. S. (2011). Feminist contributions to understanding woman abuse: Myths, controversies
and realities. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(4), 297-302.
DeKeseredy, W. S., & Schwartz, M. D. (2003). Backlash and whiplash: A critique of Canada’s General
Social Science Survey on Victimization. Online Journal of Justice Studies, 1(1).
Desmarais, S. L., Reeves, K. A., Nicholls, T. L., Telford, R. P., & Fiebert, M. S. (2012a). Prevalence of
physical violence in intimate relationships, Part 1: Rates of male and female victimization.
Partner Abuse, 3(2), 140-169.
Desmarais, S. L., Reeves, K. A., Nicholls, T. L., Telford, R. P., & Fiebert, M. S. (2012b). Prevalence of
physical violence in intimatre relationships, Part 2: Rates of male and female perpetration
Partner Abuse, 3(2), 170-198.
Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1979). Violence against wives: A case against the patriarchy. New
York, NY: Free Press.
Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1988). Research as social action: The struggle for battered women.
In K. Yllo & M. Bograd (Eds.), Feminist perspectives on wife assault (pp. 51-74). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Dragiewicz, M. (2008). Patriarchy reasserted: Fathers’ rights and anti-VAWA activism. Feminist
Criminology, 3(2), 121-144.
NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
14
Dragiewicz, M., & Lindgren, Y. (2009). The gendered nature of domestic violence: Statistical data
for lawyers considering equal protection analysis. American Journal of Gender, Social Policy
and the Law 17(2), 229-268.
Dutton, D. G. (1995a). The domestic assault of women. Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia
Press.
Dutton, D. G. (1995b). Male abusiveness in intimate relationships. Clinical Psychology Review,
15(6), 567-581.
Dutton, D. G. (2005). The domestic abuse paradigm in child custody assessments. Journal of Child
Custody, 2(4), 23-42.
Dutton, D. G. (2006). A briefer reply to Johnson. Journal of Child Custody, 3(1), 28-30.
Dutton, D. G., Hamel, J., & Aaronson, J. (2010). The gender paradigm in family court processes: Rebalancing
the scales of justice from biased social science. Journal of Child Custody, 7(1), 1-31.
Dutton, D. G., & Nicholls, T. L. (2005). The gender paradigm in domestic violence research and
theory: Part 1 –The conflict of theory and data. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10(6), 680-
714.
Follingstad, D. R., DeHart, D. D., & Green, E. P. (2004). Psychologists’ judgments of psychologically
aggressive actions when perpetrated by a husband versus a wife. Violence and Victims,
19(4), 435-452.
Gaudioisi, J. A. (2006). Child Maltreatment 2004. Retrieved from acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/stats
research/index.htm.
Goldberg, H. (1979). The new male: From self-destruction to self care. New York, NY: William
Morrow.
Gondolf, E. W. (1999). Characteristics of court-mandated batterers in four cities. Violence Against
Women, 5(11), 1277-1293.
Graham-Kevan, N., & Archer, J. (2003). Intimate terrorism and common couple violence: A test of
Johnson’s predictions in four British samples. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(11), 1247
-1270.
Henning, K., & Renauer, B. (2005). Prosecution of women arrested for intimate partner abuse. Violence
and Victims, 20(3), 361-376.
Hines, D. A. (2007). Posttraumatic stress symptoms among men who sustain partner violence: An
international multiside study of univeristy students. Psychology of Men & Masculinity,
8(4), 225-239.
Hines, D., Brown, J., & Dunning, E. (2007). Characteristics of callers to the domestic abuse hotline
for men. Journal of Family Violence, 22(2), 63-72.
Hines, D. A. & Douglas, E.M. (2010) A closer look at men who sustain intimate terroism by
women. Partner Abuse, 1(3), 286-313.
Hines, D. A., & Douglas, E. M. (2011). Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in men who sustain
intimate partner violence: A study of helpseeking and community samples. Psychology
of Men & Masculinity, 12(2), 112-127.
Jaffe, P., Lemon, N., & Poisson, S. E. (2003). Child custody & domestic violence: A call for safety
and accountability. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jaffe, P. G., Johnston, J. R., Crooks, C. V. & Bala, N. (2008). Custody disputes involving allegations
of domestic violence: Toward a differentiated view of parenting plans. Family Court Review,
46(3), 500-522.
Johnson, M. P. (1995). Patriarchal terrorism and common couple violence: Two forms of violence
against women. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57(2), 283-294.
Johnson, M. P. (2008). Intimate terrorism, violent resistance and situational couple violence.
Hanover, NH: Northeastern University Press.
Johnson, M. P., & Leone, J. M. (2005). The differential effects of intimate terrorism and situational
NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
15
couple violence: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey. Journal of
Family Issues, 26(3), 322-349.
LaRoche, D. (2005). Aspects of the context and consequences of domestic violence: Situational
couple violence and intimate terrorism in Canada in 1999. Quebec City, QC: Government
of Quebec.
Lie, G., Schilit, R., Bush, J., Montague, M., & Reyes, L. (1991). Lesbians in currently aggressive relationships:
How frequently do they report aggressive past relationships? Violence and Victims,
6(2), 121-135.
MacKinnon, C. (1989). Toward a feminist theory of state. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
McDonald, R., Jouriles, E. N., Tart, C. D., & Minze, L. C. (2009). Children’s adjustment problems in
families characterized by men’s severe violence towards women: Does other family violence
matter? Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 94-101.
Mechem, C. C., Shofer, F., Reinhard,S.S., Hornig, S., & Datner, E. (1999). History of domestic violence
among male patients presenting to an urban emergency department. Academic
Emergency Medicine, 6, 786-791.
Mills, L. G. (2003). Insult to Injury: Rethinking our response to intimate abuse. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Morse, B. (1995). Beyond the Conflict Tactics Scale: Assessing gender differences in partner violence.
Violence and Vicrims, 10(4), 251-272.
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Cottler, L. B., Works, J., Goldring, E., McEvoy, L., et al. (1987). The Diagnostic
Interview Schedule version III-A training manual. St. Louis, MO: Veterans Administration.
Russell, B. L. (2012). Effectiveness, victim safety, characteristics and enforcement of protection
order. Partner Abuse, 3(4), 531-552.
Saunders, D. G. (1986). When battered women use violence: Husband abuse or self-defense? Victims
and Violence, 1(1), 47-70.
Saunders, D. G. (1988). Wife abuse, husband abuse or mutual combat: A feminist perspective on
the empirical findings. In K. Yllo & M. Bograd (Eds.), Feminist perspectives on wife assault
(pp. 90-113). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Saunders, D. G. (1992). A typology of men who batter: Three types derived from cluster analysis.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62(2), 264-275.
Saunders, D. G. (2002). Are physical assaults by wives and girlfriends a major social problem? A review
of the literature. Violence Against Women, 8(12), 1424-1448.
Skeem, J., Schubert, C., Stowman, S., Beeson, S., Mulvey, E., Gardner, W., & Lidz, C. (2005). Gender
and risk assessment accuracy: Understanding women’s potential violence. Law and
Human Behavior, 29(2), 173-186.
Sorenson, S. B., & Taylor, C. A. (2005). Female aggression toward male intimate partners: An examination
of social norms in a community-based sample. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
29(1), 79-96.
Stets, J. E., & Straus, M. A. (1989). The marriage license as a hitting license: A comparison of assaults
in dating, cohabiting and married couples. Journal of Family Violence, 4(2), 161-180.
Stets, J., & Straus, M. A. (1992). Gender differences in reporting marital violence and its medical
and psychological consequences. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical Violence in
American Families (pp. 151-166). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Straus, M. A. (1980). Victims and aggressors in marital violence. American Behavioral Scientist,
23(5), 681-704.
Straus, M. A. (1992). The Conflict Tactics Scale and its critics: An evaluation and new data on validity
and reliability. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical Violence in American

NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
16
NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ~ ISSN 1839-7816 ~ VOL. 2, ISSUE 1, 2013, PP. 5-17
© 2013 AUSTRALIAN INSTITUTE OF MALE HEALTH AND STUDIES.
17
NEW MALE STUDIES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL (NMS) IS AN OPEN ACCESS ONLINE INTERDISCIPLINARY
JOURNAL FOR RESEARCH AND DISCUSSION OF ISSUES FACING BOYS AND MEN WORLDWIDE.
THIS JOURNAL USES OPEN JOURNAL SYSTEMS 2.3.4.0, WHICH IS OPEN SOURCE JOURNAL MANAGEMENT AND PUBLISHING
SOFTWARE DEVELOPED, SUPPORTED, AND FREELY DISTRIBUTED BY THE PUBLIC KNOWLEDGE PROJECT UNDER THE
GNU GENERAL PUBLIC LICENSE.
THIS ARTICLE HAS BEEN DOWNLOADED FROM HTTP://NEWMALESTUDIES.COM.
Families (pp. 49-73). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Straus, M. A. (1992b). Injury and frequency of assault and the “representative sample fallacy” in
measuring wife beating and child aubse. In M. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical Violence
in American Families (pp. 75-91). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Pubclishers.
Straus, M. A. (1999). The controversy over domestic violence by women: A methodological, theoretical
and sociology of science analysis (pp. 17-44). In X. B. Arriaga & S. Oskamp (Eds.),
Violence in intimate relationships (pp. 17-44). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Straus, M. A., Kaufman Kantor, G., & Moore, D. W. (1997). Change in cultural norms of approving
of marital violence from 1968 to 1994. In G. Kaufam Kantor & J. L. Jasinski (Eds.), Out of
the darkness: Contemporary perspectives on family violence (pp. 3-16). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2000). Full report of the prevalence, incidence and consequences of
violence against women: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey.
(No. NCJ- 183781). Washington, DC: US Department of Justice; Office of Justice programs,
National Institute of Justice.
Trocme, N., MacLaurin, B., Fallon, B., Daciuk, J., Billingsley, D., Tourigny, M., et al. (2001). Canadian
incidence study of reported child abuse and neglect (No. H49-151/2000E). Ottawa,
ON: Health Canada.
Vasquez, D., & Falcone, R. (1997). Cross gender violence. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 29(3),
427-429.
Whitaker, D. J., Haileyesus, T., Swahn, M., & Saltzman, L. (2007). Differences in frequency of violence
and reported injury between relationships with reciprocal and non-reciprocal intimate
partner violence. American Journal of Public Health, 97(5), 941-947.
White, K. R. & Dutton, D. G. (2013). Perceptions of female perpetrators. In B. L. Russell (Ed.), Perceptions
of female offenders: How stereotypes and social norms affect criminal justice responses
(pp. 101-116). New York, NY: Springer.
Williams, S. L., & Frieze, I. H. (2005). Patterns of violent relationships, psychological distress, and
marital satisfaction in a national sample of men and women. Sex Roles, 52(11/12), 771-784.

Don Dutton is Professor of Psychology at the University of British Columbia, Vancouver,
Canada. He may be reached at dondutton@ shaw.ca

Katherine White is a student at the University of British Columbia, Vancouver,
Canada. She may be reached at krwhite2@interchange.ubc.ca


3 comments:

  1. An excellent article, well worth reading in detail. Men who have been abused need to speak out. Thank you to the authors and to SI Victim for publishing this material here. http://domesticabusedhusband.wordpress.com/

    ReplyDelete
  2. We need a lot more facts into the public domain on the terrible plight of so many men, who not only suffer abuse from their partner but then suffer abuse from society when they try changing their lives.

    "I am a victim of domestic violence .. and it was awful"
    http://antimisandry.com/violence-against-women-men/i-am-victim-domestic-violence-awful-57428.html

    ReplyDelete
  3. This was a very interesting read! Domestic violence cases are not just an issue for females, but males too. Awareness about domestic violence as a whole can help victims come forward, regardless of gender.

    ReplyDelete